Elite Athletes Require Mental Effort in Sports

Are elite athletes like zombies who can perform on autopilot? Not according to new research.

Man sitting on rowing machine in the motion capture lab

Olaf Tufte, one of Herrebrøden's elite participants, on the ergometer in the lab. Photo: RITMO

For a long time, it has been a common belief that when you are very good at something, your body performs almost automatically - you act almost like a zombie. The Norwegian researcher Gunnar Breivik described this view in previous research.

Sports psychologist Henrik Herrebrøden also believed that automaticity was a key to success when he started his doctoral research journey in 2019.

"I may have put too much trust in athletes' verbal statements. In several studies, athletes have claimed that they did not think about anything, or that they were relatively relaxed, while performing. On the one hand, this seems quite sensible and very pleasant. The problem is that it probably does not give a thorough and truthful picture of a typical sports performance," Herrebrøden says.

He recently completed his PhD at the RITMO Centre for Interdisciplinary Studies in Rhythm, Time and Motion, University of Oslo. Herrebrøden investigated cognition and attentional processes in rowers.

"Ergometer rowing may not seem like an activity that requires attention. But it does!"

Henrik Herrebrøden
In his studies, Henrik Herrebrøden found that ergometer rowing takes attention and effort, even in elite rowers. 15 September, 2023 Herrebrøden will defend his PhD dissertation Attention and Effort in Elite Athletes: A Case Against Automaticity. Photo: Silje Pileberg

Elites had the largest pupil dilations

Herrebrøden and colleagues tested 18 rowers in the lab: nine from the Norwegian national team and nine recreational rowers. All of them wore eye-tracking glasses measuring pupil size and blinking. They used a rowing ergometer and were instructed to row with certain levels of speed, while a monitor told them how fast they were rowing.

"It is not easy to measure brain activity when a person is physically active, but our eyes can give us some hints," says Herrebrøden.

For example, pupil size can say something about how intensively we use our attention.

"When comparing the elites with those at a lower level, we saw that the elite rowers demonstrated significantly greater increase in pupil size. This surprised us."

Close-up image of eyes
Eye of an elite rower while sitting still (left) and while using a rowing ergometer (right). Photo: RITMO

The elite mind is not just calm

Humans' pupil size can vary between ca. 1.5 and 8 millimetres. Increased pupil size may indicate increased activity in the cerebral cortex, the outer layer of the cerebrum, which is involved in thinking, interpretation of sensory impressions, planning, and execution of complex actions.

Herrebrøden himself used to play volleyball at the elite level.

"When we do something we are good at, and it goes well, it can feel as if the mind is completely calm - everything just flows. But in my PhD, I got to observe some of the world's best rowers while they performed well-known actions, and they seemed to use intensive attention at least as much as rowers at lower levels."

Subjective versus objective effort

So, could it be that we make intensive use of our brainpower without noticing?

"It can happen! Sometimes we can become completely absorbed in what we do. The performance feels easy and pleasant, and we use a lot of mental effort without being consciously aware of it. We often use the term "flow" to describe this experience, and flow usually occurs when we are challenged within the limits of our capacity."

However, the opposite phenomenon can also occur. Sometimes the activity feels effortful, while you may actually not put in a lot of effort. Researchers saw this in car racing simulation study. When driving conditions were very difficult, participants claimed to invest a lot of effort while physiological measurements indicated that they did not.

In sum, there is no one-to-one relationship between the effort we feel that we invest and the measurable, objective effort that we actually seem to invest.

Math problems led to declined rowing performance

During some of the trials, Herrebrøden wanted to give the participants a mental challenge. While rowing they were presented with math problems through loudspeakers, for instance, 36 + 3 and 15 x 16. They had ten seconds to respond to each problem.

"We saw that the trials involving math problems led to pupil dilations, as has been demonstrated in previous research. In our study, we also saw that the dual tasking affected the rowing performance negatively," Herrebrøden says.

While solving the math problems, the participants rowed with more variable speed levels and shorter stroke lengths, as compared to conditions without math problems.

"It seems like optimal sports performance requires attention. If you perform several attention-demanding tasks at the same time, your brain will look for shortcuts and prioritize. Our participants may have sacrificed precise speed control, as well as the long and beautiful strokes on the ergometer, in order to deal with the math problems while rowing."

Three minutes without blinking

Herrebrøden also investigated how many times the rowers blinked per trial.

"During single task rowing, with no math problems, the participants usually did not blink much. Some could row for three minutes without blinking! This could be a sign of intense concentration on what they were doing and the monitor in front of them."

The producers of the short film "Blink of an eye" observed a similar finding in 2013, when filming the Norwegian alpine skier Aksel Lund Svindal. He went downhill for two minutes - all the way to the bottom of the hill - without blinking. Normally humans blink approximately every third second.

When the rowers had to dual task, by rowing and solving math problems at the same time, the blink rates increased.

"A possible explanation is that the rowers had to "shut the world out" for a moment and concentrate on calculations within their minds. One way to disconnect from the external world is to close your eyes, or blink more often."

Sport scientists have yet to uncover all that goes on inside the elite athlete's mind.

"Overall, it seems like we "come alive" when we play sports. Our hearts beat faster, and our pupils get bigger. We are only starting to explore how such physiological characteristics are linked to athletes' cognition, and there is still a lot we do not know," Herrebrøden says.

A cognitively demanding activity

Through conversations with elite athletes, Herrebrøden has realized that an elite sports performance is more cognitively demanding and nuanced than many people may think. For an outside observer, rowing may seem fairly straightforward. Elite rowers, on the other hand, could talk for minutes about their thinking and strategies during a race.

"It might be comparable to wine tasting. If you have not tasted red wine before, most red wines may taste the same and you probably will not notice much nuance. For a wine connoisseur, on the other hand, each new red wine represents a world of different flavours, textures, smells and so on. Likewise, a rowing stroke cycle may offer more details to an elite rower's attention than a novice," Herrebrøden suggests.

Whether or not this can explain the large pupil dilations found in Herrebrøden's elite participants, is not clear.

"I do not think the elite rowers would have spent mental effort on rowing if it did not help their performance. My best explanation is that the elites pay attention and process information, such as feedback from their bodies or contextual cues in the performance environment, in ways that make them more adaptable and effective. However, they may be so used to this attentive processing that they do not find it draining or particularly demanding."

Rower carrying the Norwegian national team's oars in the Olympics.
Covid-19 caused disruptions for sporting event organizers and researchers alike, but Herrebrøden was able to complete his data collection and the Olympic Games were held in Tokyo. This is national team rower Oscar Helvig ahead of the Games in 2021. Photo: Balint Czucz Photography

References

Henrik Herrebrøden, Thomas Espeseth og Laura Bishop (2023): "Mental Effort in Elite and Nonelite Rowers"

Henrik Herrebrøden, Alexander Refsum Jensenius, Thomas Espeseth, Laura Bishop, Jonna Katariina Vuoskoski (2023): "Cognitive load causes kinematic changes in both elite and non-elite rowers"

/University of Oslo Public Release. This material from the originating organization/author(s) might be of the point-in-time nature, and edited for clarity, style and length. Mirage.News does not take institutional positions or sides, and all views, positions, and conclusions expressed herein are solely those of the author(s).View in full here.